下載本文檔
版權(quán)說明:本文檔由用戶提供并上傳,收益歸屬內(nèi)容提供方,若內(nèi)容存在侵權(quán),請進(jìn)行舉報或認(rèn)領(lǐng)
文檔簡介
1、The Problems with Activation-Synthesis TheoryG. William DomhoffUniversity of California, Santa CruzNOTE: This is an unpublished paper. If you use this article in research, please use the following citation: Domhoff, G. W. (2000). The Problems with Activation-Synthesis Theory. Retrieved October 28, 2
2、006 from the World Wide Web: For a more recent overview of the debate surrounding activation-synthesis theory, see Refocusing the neurocognitive approach to dreams: A critique of the Hobson versus Solms debate (2005). AbstractDream content is more coherent, consistent over time, and continuous with
3、waking emotional concerns than the activation-synthesis theory would predict. It cannot easily accommodate the fact that children under age 5 have infrequent and bland dreams even though they have normal Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep. It is not consistent with the fact that there are patients with
4、forebrain lesions who do not dream even though they have REM sleep. A neurocognitive perspective should focus on the forebrain system of dream generation and begin with the findings on dream content in adults and the developmental nature of dreaming in children. IntroductionA large body of findings
5、with the Hall/Van de Castle coding system (Domhoff, 1996; Domhoff, 1999a; Hall & Van de Castle, 1966) shows that dreams are more coherent, consistent over time for both individuals and groups, and continuous with past and present waking emotional concerns than the activation-synthesis theory, wi
6、th its emphasis on brainstem-driven bizarreness, can accommodate. (Hobson, 1988; Hobson, Pace-Schott, & Stickgold, 2000; Hobson, Stickgold, & Pace-Schott, 1998) In addition, Foulkes' (1982; 1999) laboratory discovery of low levels of dreaming until ages 9-11 joins with Solms' (1997)
7、findings with brain-lesioned patients in demonstrating that REM sleep is insufficient for dreaming. A new neurocognitive theory of dreaming therefore should begin with the hypothesis that Foulkes' developmental findings may correlate with the maturation of the forebrain system of dream generatio
8、n first uncovered through creative neuropsychological detective work by Solms. In addition, the findings with the Hall/Van de Castle system on the lifelong persistence of various kinds of negative dream content suggest there is a "repetition dimension" in people's dream life (Domhoff,
9、1993; Domhoff, 1996) that may relate to the temporal-limbic and frontal-limbic origins of dreaming in Solms' model. The Brainstem/Bizarreness CommitmentHobson et. al. present interesting ideas that may explain away much of the "dreaming" in NREM sleep. However, they do concede there is
10、 enough dreamlike mental activity in NREM sleep to challenge the strict equation of dreaming and the REM stage of sleep, especially late in the sleep period. The empirical dream psychologists who abandoned the REM sleep/dreaming equation decades ago in the face of contradictory evidence summarized b
11、y Berger (1967; 1969), Foulkes (1966; 1967), and Hall (1967) did not ask for much more than what is now granted in these articles. The Hobson group is wrong to chastise psychologists for focusing on the cognitive level when the constant changes in their own model show that their comprehensive mind-b
12、rain isomorphism is extremely premature. It is regrettable that Hobson et. al took so long to broaden their theory in the face of contradictory evidence available long ago (Vogel, 1978), but it is possible that the "state" transition at sleep onset and the greater activation late in a slee
13、p period explain much dreamlike NREM mentation. The disappointment is their continuing brainstem commitment. In the face of the new and old findings synthesized by Solms to show that brainstem activation is not sufficient for dreaming, and in some unknown percentage of cases may not even be necessar
14、y, it would seem that research relating the forebrain system to many different aspects of dream content should now be the primary focus of mind-brain isomorphists. Hobson et. al. justify their desire to keep the brainstem at the forefront of their theory on the basis of a commitment to a mind-brain
15、isomorphism. However, this insistence also may be due to their strong belief that dreams are bizarre and discontinuous, although one of their own studies reported "discontinuities" in only 34% of 200 dreams (Rittenhouse, Stickgold, & Hobson, 1994). Most others who have studied large sa
16、mples of dream reports from groups and individuals see dreams as even more like realistic (Dorus, Dorus, & Rechtschaffen, 1971; Foulkes, 1985; Snyder, 1970; Strauch & Meier, 1996). For example, Hall (1966) concluded that only 10% of 815 home and laboratory reports from 14 adult males had at
17、least one "unusual element," using a scale that can be found in Domhoff (1996). In studies comparing REM reports to samples of waking thought collected from participants reclining in a darkened room, the waking samples were rated as more dreamlike (Reinsel, Antrobus, & Wollman, 1992; R
18、einsel, Wollman, & Antrobus, 1986). To support their focus on brainstem activation and the bizarre nature of dream content, Hobson et. al. have to challenge several different sets of impressive findings. First, they reject Foulkes' (1982; 1999) conclusions on the low levels of REM dreaming i
19、n young children with the claim that these children are not able to communicate in words about their dreams. But Foulkes' data show that the rate of recall correlates with visuospatial skills, and that there are older children with good communication skills and poor visuospatial skills who do no
20、t recall very many dreams in the laboratory. It is more likely that young children do not dream often or well by adult standards, a conclusion favoring a cognitive theory of dreams. Hobson et. al. reject Foulkes' findings on the banality of the few dreams his young participants did report by say
21、ing the laboratory situation is not conducive to typical dreaming, but Foulkes (1979; 1996; 1999) already has answered that claim very effectively. More generally, they overstate the differences between home and laboratory dreams. This is shown most recently in a reanalysis using effect sizes (Domho
22、ff & Schneider, 1999) with the original codings from the most comprehensive study of this issue, which was carried out by Hall (1966) with 11 young adult male participants who each spent three to four consecutive weeks sleeping in a laboratory bedroom in a house in a residential neighborhood. Ho
23、bson et. al. denigrate the findings on the everyday nature of most dream content by saying that psychological measurement has not been adequate, but they have not demonstrated that their evolving rating scales for the slippery concept of bizarreness can be used reliably across laboratories. Furtherm
24、ore, they ignore most of the findings with the Hall/Van de Castle system, which has shown high reliability when used by researchers in many different countries and produced results that have been replicated several times (Domhoff, 1996; Domhoff, 1999b) However, Hobson et. al. do note the Hall/Van de
25、 Castle findings on emotion in dreams, which anticipate their own findings of more negative than positive emotions, more reports of emotions in women's dreams, and no gender differences in the distribution of emotions (Merritt, Stickgold, Pace-Schott, Williams, & Hobson, 1994). In their effo
26、rt to emphasize differences between REM and NREM reports, Hobson et. al. argue against any control for length of report. In so doing they do not take seem to realize this problem is handled without loss of data by the indicators based on percentages and ratios that are now standard in the Hall/Van d
27、e Castle system (Domhoff, 1999b; Schneider & Domhoff, 1995-2000). Strong support for the use of the Hall/Van de Castle content indicators in resolving disputes about the nature of REM and NREM reports is provided by a study Hall carried out three decades ago, but that was only recently reported
28、by Domhoff and Schneider (1999). When NREM reports from early and late in the sleep period were compared with REM reports, several of the usual differences appeared. For example, the "cognitive activities percent" (the number of cognitive activities divided by the total number of all activ
29、ities) was 20% in NREM reports, but only 11% in REM reports. Conversely, the "verbal activity percent" was 37% in REM reports, but only 22% in REM reports. However, the NREM reports from after the third REM period of the night were more similar to REM reports than early NREM reports on a s
30、ummary measure for a wide range of Hall/Van de Castle categories. These results are consistent with the recent theorizing by Hobson et. al. Hobson et. al. call for studies of dreams at home to obtain a more realistic sample of dream content, but they overlook the replicated longitudinal results with
31、 the Hall/Van de Castle system, which show that dream content can be constant for individual adults over years and decades, something that might not be expected if dreaming is as chaotic and bizarre as they claim (Domhoff, 1996). One of these longitudinal studies showed that the dreams of "the Engine Man," used by Hobson
溫馨提示
- 1. 本站所有資源如無特殊說明,都需要本地電腦安裝OFFICE2007和PDF閱讀器。圖紙軟件為CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.壓縮文件請下載最新的WinRAR軟件解壓。
- 2. 本站的文檔不包含任何第三方提供的附件圖紙等,如果需要附件,請聯(lián)系上傳者。文件的所有權(quán)益歸上傳用戶所有。
- 3. 本站RAR壓縮包中若帶圖紙,網(wǎng)頁內(nèi)容里面會有圖紙預(yù)覽,若沒有圖紙預(yù)覽就沒有圖紙。
- 4. 未經(jīng)權(quán)益所有人同意不得將文件中的內(nèi)容挪作商業(yè)或盈利用途。
- 5. 人人文庫網(wǎng)僅提供信息存儲空間,僅對用戶上傳內(nèi)容的表現(xiàn)方式做保護(hù)處理,對用戶上傳分享的文檔內(nèi)容本身不做任何修改或編輯,并不能對任何下載內(nèi)容負(fù)責(zé)。
- 6. 下載文件中如有侵權(quán)或不適當(dāng)內(nèi)容,請與我們聯(lián)系,我們立即糾正。
- 7. 本站不保證下載資源的準(zhǔn)確性、安全性和完整性, 同時也不承擔(dān)用戶因使用這些下載資源對自己和他人造成任何形式的傷害或損失。
最新文檔
- 二零二五年度護(hù)校與養(yǎng)老機構(gòu)合作服務(wù)合同3篇
- 女生節(jié)活動策劃方案(3篇)
- 中小學(xué)校實驗室內(nèi)部管理制度范文(二篇)
- 2025年度物流運輸安全環(huán)保服務(wù)協(xié)議范本3篇
- 液壓銑床課程設(shè)計摘要
- 財務(wù)分析圖表課程設(shè)計
- 平路機安全操作規(guī)程范文(2篇)
- 二零二五年度房地產(chǎn)租賃權(quán)包銷合同3篇
- 2025年上半年安全員工作總結(jié)(3篇)
- 2024年滬教版高三歷史上冊階段測試試卷
- 2024年河南省公務(wù)員考試《行測》真題及答案解析
- 徐州醫(yī)科大學(xué)附屬醫(yī)院
- 自動化立體庫貨架驗收報告
- 消防系統(tǒng)工程質(zhì)量控制資料檢查記錄
- 中藥封包療法操作規(guī)范
- 浙江產(chǎn)業(yè)帶分布情況
- 道岔主要幾何尺寸表
- 柳宗元毛筆楷書字帖
- 纖力玻璃鋼管道厚度,重量一覽表
- 新浪網(wǎng)刪貼申請文檔 (個人)
- 低溫乙烯罐內(nèi)罐預(yù)冷過程溫度急降原因探討
評論
0/150
提交評論